In
a widely-reported study released last year, two researchers over at the
University of Missouri, Columbia tested the effects on plant defenses of the
vibrations caused by a caterpillar chewing on a leaf. Although much of the
reporting fell prey to the temptation to claim the plants “heard” the chewing
and responded, the real answer is both more complicated and more interesting. I
had the opportunity to attend a talk Drs. Appel and Cocroft gave at Washington
University a few months ago where I learned more than I could have extracted
from their paper, published in Oecologia, alone.
Sound waves are longitudinal. Insect vibrations are transverse |
Dr. Cocroft studies insect communication, especially the ability
of insects to find mates and prey by sensing the vibrations of other insects on
a plant. Like sound, the information is encoded in vibrational waves passing
through a substance. Instead of a pressure wave like sound that varies in the
same direction of travel—a longitudinal wave—insect vibrations on plants are
transverse waves, moving up and down like a wave on the ocean (see figure).
We
could never hear these kinds of waves ourselves, but their frequency can be
directly translated to sounds we can hear. Cocroft played a number of humming
soundscapes recorded with a laser on a wild prairie—the result of hundreds or
thousands of insects communicating silently on stalks of grass. A plant, Cocroft
noted, is a great conductor for these vibrations, flexible yet strong. His
field studies how insects benefit from communicating this way, but he joined
forces with Appel to ask: Do plants respond to the vibrations of insect
herbivores in an adaptive way?
One major defense that plants have against pests is producing
noxious compounds to deter feeding. Appel and Cocroft hypothesized that
Arabidopsis plants would produce more defense compounds if they were exposed to
the vibrations of herbivorous insects before actually being attacked. This
effect is called priming, and could help defend against a second wave of insect
damage.
To test this, the researchers first used lasers to record the
vibrations of caterpillars allowed to eat the leaves of Arabidopsis plants. To
play the vibrations back to undamaged plants, Cocroft attached leaves to tiny
pistons driven, essentially, by speakers, ones that could replicate the
vibrations of an insect chewing. Then caterpillars were allowed to feed on
either the leaf that was vibrated or another, untouched leaf.
Both
vibrated and distant leaves responded more vigorously to caterpillar attack
than leaves on untouched plants. The plants that were primed by recorded
caterpillar vibrations produced more glucosinolates, or mustard oils, than
those of unvibrated plants. This is evidence of an adaptive response to insect
vibrations, but leaves open the possibility that any vibration encouraged plant defenses.
To see if the effect really was specific to the herbivorous
caterpillars, Appel and Cocroft played back vibrations of harmless insects,
wind, or caterpillars on different plants and again measured defense
compounds—this time anthocyanins, responsible for the deep reds and purples of
many plants. Only caterpillar vibrations could prime plants to increase their
defense response to herbivory; wind and the neutral insects had no effect.
One important caveat: although the researchers looked for an
effect of vibrations alone, they found none. Only vibrations plus actual insect
feeding induced higher defenses; the plants were primed for future attack, but
vibrations alone made no difference. Of course, a real insect is more than just
its vibrations. Herbivore attack is a physical, chemical, and auditory assault,
and plants likely respond to each stimulus in different ways.
But how are plants able to sense the vibrations of caterpillars,
and even differentiate them from similar sounds in nature? It’s entirely
unknown. A very good candidate is a diverse group of proteins bound together by
their responsiveness to physical forces—mechanoreceptors. These proteins can
signal within a cell in response to vibration or touch and are potentially
behind the priming effect that Appel and Cocroft observed.
In
fact, to test this, the Haswell lab is working with Appel and Cocroft to see
if our favorite mechanosensitive ion channels are part of the
vibrational-response pathway. I got to see Liz’s face pop up in the corner at
the end of their presentation over on the medical campus as they told us that
work was underway. We’ll just have to wait to find out.
[A version of this post first appeared on my lab's blog]
No comments:
Post a Comment